One of the latest
papers to come out on the science of psychedelics, published last month in the
Journal of Psychopharmacology, made quite a splash when it suggested that
hallucinogens could have a “protective effect” on intimate partner violence (IPV,
aka domestic violence).
I was asked my opinion on this. I had an opinion or two.
I was asked my opinion on this. I had an opinion or two.
In the study, the
researchers took a sample of male jail inmates – jailed for any number of
reasons – and at some point during their jail time, asked them what drugs they
had done in the past. Turns out all of them had used drugs in the past, but
those who listed hallucinogens among those drugs were less likely to be
re-incarcerated on IPV charges following their release than those who hadn’t
used hallucinogens. To be specific, 27% of the hallucinogen-use group were
arrested for later IPV, compared with 42% of the group that reported no
hallucinogen use. (That is not to say inmates who had used hallucinogens
weren’t also re-incarcerated, on other
charges. But the difference emerged specifically for IPV.)
This comes as a bit of
a surprise to the scientific community (although surely not to anyone who has
ever tried a hallucinogen or is aware of their therapeutic benefits), as it’s considered
well-established that drug use correlates with violence, and conversely, treatment
for drug use decreases violence. But rather than follow this pattern,
hallucinogens seem to be in a category of their own, doing the exact opposite. The
results make the point that hallucinogens probably shouldn’t be lumped in with
other drugs, and once again point to the potential benefits of hallucinogen
use, not just for the individual but for society more generally.
Of course, my critical
scientist mind immediately reminds me that correlation is not causation, and that
the relationship could be mediated by any number of things. Maybe the kind of
person who is drawn to hallucinogens is less prone to violence to begin with?
Or less likely to have a partner? Or maybe hallucinogen use makes you less
interested in drugs that are known to instigate violence, like alcohol or meth,
so it’s not the presence of hallucinogens per
se, but rather the absence of violence-promoting drugs? Or maybe it’s a socio-cultural
phenomenon, seeing how 2/3 of the hallucinogen-using sample was white?
Who knows, the study
doesn’t go into reasons for the relationship – although they did successfully control
for the potential effects of ethnicity/socio-cultural factors, psychopathic
personality, and alcoholism: the relationship held even when those factors were
taken out of the equation. Nonetheless, we only know that this relationship exists, not why. But that’s already
something.
It’s tempting to
speculate, though, that this fits with the growing narrative of hallucinogens’ therapeutic benefits - and this is the angle favoured by the authors. For example, they
remind the reader that psilocybin was recently shown in small clinical trials
to help people quit drinking and smoking, and that studies of LSD-assisted
psychotherapy, dating back as far as the 60s (when LSD was still legal), showed
positive personality changes, in addition to proving helpful in treating alcoholism. They also note that even in healthy people, hallucinogens have been
shown to increase ‘interpersonal regard,’ intimate emotional communication,
empathy, and ‘broad-minded tolerance of others.’
Not mentioned in their
summary are more recent findings that ayahuasca/DMT increases a person’s ‘mindfulness capacity,’ bringing their ability to ‘observe their mind without judging’ to a
level reported by experienced meditators; that a single administration of LSD increases ‘suggestibility’ (susceptibility to suggestion, which can increase
treatment effectiveness) and both psilocybin and LSD increased optimism and
openness for over a year, suggesting a lasting personality change; that a
single administration of psilocybin changes how people process social information; and that in general, hallucinogens re-organise how brain networks communicate, and that this correlates with a sense of ‘ego-dissolution,’ or
one-ness with the world. Not to mention the long-documented ability of MDMA to
induce feelings of interpersonal closeness and sociability. Seems that any one
of those factors would make a person less likely to attack those around them,
no?
So why is it that the
authors’ conclusions are being questioned left and right? Their conclusions are
relatively unassuming: 1. that hallucinogens are an exception to the rule regarding
substance use and criminal behaviour; and 2. that further investigation is
warranted into the potential of hallucinogens to prevent violent behaviour.
Neither is particularly controversial.
Part of the problem is
that none of this careful language and nuance is reflected in articles like the
one by The Mirror, who mistakenly explains this “sensational study” in terms of
“hallucinogen users and their sober compatriots” (none of the inmates were
sober) and focuses almost exclusively on the treatment implications. No wonder
publications like politics.co.uk react negatively, dismissing the entire study
as flawed for having ‘medicalised’ and thus excused violent behaviour (it
didn’t).
These articles are
making me realise that if we want to normalise and advance this conversation,
we need to do much more than simply publicise study results; we need to come to
an agreement on the currently delicate and tenuous state of psychedelic science,
and the language around it. (Nevermind agreeing to perhaps read the actual
studies, rather than misunderstanding and misinterpreting them based on an
article by The Mirror.)
Look, psychedelic
science is a hella controversial field. To catch on, and to gain the legality
and legitimacy it needs in order to build its evidence base, it needs to
overturn people’s deeply engrained beliefs about drugs, and overcome legal,
academic, and practical obstacles at every turn. (And btw, it’s OK if you don’t
believe it or think the field as a whole is hoakey – all the more reason to
support the science trying to explore it. Let the evidence corroborate [or
change] your opinion.) Anyway, as it
stands, psychedelic science is having a hard enough time, from funding
shortages and publication bias to negative publicity and a terrible reputation
to overcome; let’s not use it as a spring-board for arguments that could just
as easily be made about less controversial examples (violent behaviour, for
example, has been treated for ages with antidepressants and cognitive-behavioural
approaches; plus we’ve been treating behavioural problems for ages, from
gambling addiction to eating disorders – so this was a terrible example to pick
to make a point about the ‘exculpatory’ nature of treatment).
As for the language around
its therapeutic potential, we should agree on the meaning of ‘treatment.’ The
politics.co.uk article’s argument comes from a place where treatment = cure.
We’re trying to give people LSD to “cure” the “illness” of partner violence, to
medicate it out of people while they passively enjoy the benefits of having
been given a “pass” for their behaviour. But no one is saying that. That’s not
what the study is about. That’s not what treatment is about. Treatment isn’t
about medical care, or fixing a problem without the person having to do
anything. It’s about throwing every tool in our arsenal at every cause we know
of for a behaviour in an effort to prevent that behaviour manifesting again.
Why wouldn’t it be in everyone’s
interest to have more tools in our arsenal – and especially one that gets at
the very core of the humanity we want such offenders to cultivate?
No comments:
Post a Comment